Literacy in a post-truth world
DOI:
https://doi.org/10.17058/signo.v47i90.17870Palabras clave:
Literacy, Logical reasoning, Critical thinking, Conspiracy theoriesResumen
: This theoretical paper examines whether acquiring better literacy skills may help people to be less vulnerable to various types of information disorders such as fake news or conspiracy theories. We start this venture by looking, first, at the relationships between vulnerability to information disorders and literacy (or, more generally, formal education), and second, at the literacy-induced transfer effects on cognitive skills. Then we will comment on some interesting findings made within the framework of an experimental project in which we examined subliterate adults and adolescents. Grounded on these results, we conjecture on the relationships between literacy, logical reasoning, critical thinking and endorsement of conspiracy theories.
Descargas
Citas
AARNIO, K; LINDEMAN, M. Paranormal beliefs, education, and thinking styles. Personality and Individual Differences, v. 39, n. 7, p. 1227–1236, 2005. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2005.04.009
ALLCOTT, H.; GENTZKOW, M. Social media and fake news in the 2016 election. Journal of Economic Perspectives, v. 31, n. 2, p. 211–236, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1257/jep.31.2.211
ARKE, E. T.; PRIMACK, B. A. Quantifying media literacy: Development, reliability, and validity of a new measure. Educational Media International, v. 46, n. 1, p. 53–65, 2009.
https://doi.org/10.1080/09523980902780958.
ARNAL, C.; KOLINSKY, R.; KLEIN, O. Conspiracy: an illness for poor thinkers? | Le complotisme: maladie des mal-pensants? Cahiers de Psychologie Clinique, v. 58, n. 1, p. 111–131, 2022. https://doi.org/10.3917/cpc.058.0111
ARUM, R.; GAMORAN, A.; SHAVIT, Y. More inclusion than diversion: Expansion, differentiation, and market structure in higher education. In Stratification in higher education — a comparative study (pp. 1-35). Stanford: Stanford University Press, 2007.
BEEDIE, C. J.; FOAD, A. J. The placebo effect in sports performance: a brief review. Sports Medicine, v. 39, n. 4, p. 313–329, 2009.
BOLTZMANN, M.; MÜNTE, T.; MOHAMMADI, B.; RÜSSELER, J. Functional and structural neural plasticity effects of literacy acquisition in adulthood. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, v. 30, p. 97–107, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1024/1016-264X/a000254
BOLTZMANN, M.; RÜSSELER, J.; YE, Z.; MÜNTE, T. F. Learning to read in adulthood: an evaluation of a literacy program for functionally illiterate adults in Germany. Problems
of Education in the 21st Century, v. 51, p. 33–46, 2013.
BULAJIC, A.; DESPOTOVIĆ, M.; LACHMANN, T. (2019). Understanding functional illiteracy from a policy, adult education, and cognition point of view: Towards a joint referent framework. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, v. 30, n. 2, p. 109–122, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1024/1016-264X/a000255.
BRASIL DE FATO. Programa Bem Viver debate impacto da pandemia na alfabetização de crianças. [The Living Well program debates the impact of the pandemic on children’s literacy]. February 14, 2022. https://podcast.brasildefato.com.br/2022/02/programa-bem-viver-debate-impacto-da-pandemia-na-alfabetizacao-de-criancas/
BRONNER, G. La démocratie des crédules. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 2013.
BROTHERTON, R.; FRENCH, C. C.; PICKERING, A. D. Measuring Belief in Conspiracy Theories: The Generic Conspiracist Beliefs Scale. Frontiers in Psychology, v. 4, n. 279, 2013. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00279
BRUM, E. How a homophobic, misogynist, racist ‘thing’ could be Brazil’s next president. The Guardian, 2018. Available at: https://www.theguardian.com/commentisfree/2018/oct/06/homophobic-mismogynist-racist-brazil-jair-bolsonaro. Accessed in: August 15, 2022.
BULAJIC, A.; DESPOTOVIĆ, M.; LACHMANN, T. Understanding functional illiteracy from a policy, adult education, and cognition point of view: Towards a joint referent framework. Zeitschrift für Neuropsychologie, v. 30, n. 2, p. 109–122, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1024/1016-264X/a000255
CASTRO-CALDAS, A. et al. The illiterate brain. Learning to read and write during childhood influences the functional organization of the adult brain. Brain, v. 121, n. 6, p. 1053–1063, 1998. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/121.6.1053
CORRÊA, V. O objeto direto nulo no português do Brasil. Dissertação (Mestrado em Linguística). Unicamp, 1991
DEMOULIN, C.; KOLINSKY, R. Does learning to read shape verbal working memory? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, v. 23, n. 3, p. 703–722, 2016. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0956-7
DOUGLAS, K. M. et al. Someone is pulling the strings: Hypersensitive agency detection and belief in conspiracy theories. Thinking and Reasoning, v. 22, n. 1, p. 57–77, 2016. https://doi.org/10.1080/13546783.2015.1051586
ENNIS, R. H. A concept of critical thinking. Harvard Educational Review, v. 22, n. 1, p. 81–111, 1962.
ENNIS, R. H. A conception of rational thinking. In J. Coombs (Ed), Philosophy of education 1979. Proceedings of the thirty-fifth annual meeting of the Philosophy of Education Society (pp. 3–30). Normal, Illinois: Philosophy of Education Society, 1980.
ENNIS, R. H.; MILLMAN, J.; TOMKO, T. N. Cornell Critical Thinking Tests Level X and Level Z Manual, 5ª edition. Seaside, CA: The Critical Thinking Co., 2005.
FACIONE, P. A.; FACIONE, N. C.; GIANCARLO, C. A. The disposition toward critical thinking: its character, measurement, and relationship to critical thinking skill. Informal Logic, v. 20, n.1, p. 61–84, 2000. https://doi.org/10.22329/il.v20i1.2254
FARLEY, M. J.; ELMORE, P. B. The relationship of reading comprehension to critical thinking skills, cognitive ability, and vocabulary for a sample of underachieving college freshmen. Education and Psychological Measurement, v. 52, n. 4, p. 921–931, 1992. https://doi.org/10.1177/0013164492052004014
FLYNN, D. J.; NYHAN, B.; REIFLER, J. The nature and origins of misperceptions: Understanding false and unsupported beliefs about politics. Political Psychology, v. 38, p. 127–50, 2017.
FOSTER, C.; FRIEDEN, J. Crisis of trust: Socio-economic determinants of Europeans’ confidence in government. European Union Politics, v. 18, n. 4, p. 511–535, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1177/1465116517723499
FRANKFURT, H. G. On bullshit. Princeton, N. J.: Princeton University Press, 2005.
FRANKS, B.; BANGERTER, A.; BAUER, M. W.; HALL, M.; NOORT, M. C. Beyond “Monologicality”? Exploring Conspiracist Worldviews. Frontiers in Psychology, v. 8, p. 861, 2017. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00861
FREDERIK, S. Cognitive reflection and decision making. Journal of Economic Perspectives, v. 19, n. 4, p. 25–42, 2005. https://doi.org/10.1257/089533005775196732
FREEMAN, D.; BENTALL, R. P. The concomitants of conspiracy concerns. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, v. 52, n. 5, p. 595–604, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127-017-1354-4
GABRIEL, R.; KOLINSKY, R.; MORAIS, J. A literacia no desenvolvimento da memória de curto prazo. [Literacy in short-term memory development]. In M. Maia (Ed), Psicolinguística: Diversidades, Interfaces e Aplicações (pp. 55-70). São Paulo: Contexto, 2022.
GABRIEL, R.; MORAIS, J.; KOLINSKY, R. A aprendizagem da leitura e suas implicações sobre a memória e a cognição. [Learning to read and its implications on memory and cognition]. Ilha do Desterro, v. 59, n. 1, p. 61–78, 2016. https://doi.org/10.5007/2175-8026.2016v69n1p61
GEORGIOU, N.; DELFABBRO, P.; BALZAN, R. Conspiracy beliefs in the general population: The importance of psychopathology, cognitive style and educational attainment. Personality and Individual Differences, v. 151, 109521, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.109521
GOERTZEL, T. Belief in Conspiracy Theories. Political Psychology, v. 15, n. 4, p. 731–742, 1994. https://doi.org/10.2307/3791630
GEORGIOU, N.; DELFABBRO, P.; BALZAN, R. COVID-19-related conspiracy beliefs and their relationship with perceived stress and preexisting conspiracy beliefs. Personality and Individual Differences, v. 166, 110201, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2020.110201
GOUBIN, S.; HOOGHE, M. The Effect of Inequality on the Relation Between Socioeconomic Stratification and Political Trust in Europe. Social Justice Research, v. 33, n. 2, p. 219–247, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11211-020-00350-z
GUESS, A.; NYHAN, B.; REIFLER, J. Exposure to untrustworthy websites in the 2016 U.S. election. Nature Human Behaviour, v. 4, n. 5, p. 472–480, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-020-0833-x
HENRICH, J.; HEINE, S. J.; NORENZAYAN, A. The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, v. 33, n. 2–3, p. 61–83, 2010. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
Huettig, F.; Pickering, M. J. Literacy advantages beyond reading: Prediction of spoken language. Trends in Cognitive Science, v. 23, p. 464–475, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.03.008
IMHOFF, R.; BRUDER, M. Speaking (Un-)Truth to Power: Conspiracy Mentality as a Generalised Political Attitude. European Journal of Personality, v. 28, n. 1, p. 25–43, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.1930
KATO, M. A.; CYRINO, S. L.; CORRÊA, V. R. Brazilian Portuguese and the recovery of lost clitics through schooling. Minimalist Inquiries into Child and Adult Language Acquisition, Studies on Language Acquisition [SOLA], v. 35 p. 245–272, 2009. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110215359.2.245
KEELEY, B. L. Of Conspiracy Theories. The Journal of Philosophy, v. 96, n. 3, p. 109–126, 1999. https://doi.org/10.2307/2564659
KOLINSKY, R. How learning to read influences language and cognition. In A. Pollatsek & R. Treiman (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Reading (pp. 377-393). New York: Oxford University Press, 2015. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199324576.013.29
KOLINSKY, R.; GABRIEL, R., DEMOULIN, C., GREGORY, M. M.; CARVALHO, K. S.; MORAIS, J. The influence of age, schooling, literacy, and socioeconomic status on serial-order memory. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, v. 4, p. 343–365, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-020-00056-3
KOLINSKY, R.; MONTEIRO-PLANTIN, R. S.; MENGARDA, E. J. GRIMM-CABRAL, L.; SCLIAR-CABRAL, L.; MORAIS, J. How formal education and literacy impact on the content and structure of semantic categories. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, v. 3, n. 3–4, p. 106–121, 2014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2014.08.001
KOLINSKY, R.; MORAIS, J. The worries of wearing literate glasses. L’Année Psychologique/Topics in Cognitive Psychology, v. 118, n. 4, p. 321–347, 2018
KOSMIDIS, M. H.; ZAFIRI, M.; POLITIMOU, N. Literacy versus formal schooling: Influence on working memory. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, v. 26, n. 7, p. 575–582, 2011. https://doi.org/10.1093/arclin/acr063
LAFONTAINE, D.; QUITTRE, V.; CRÉPIN, F. Les compétences des jeunes de 15 ans en sciences, en mathématiques et en lecture. Résultats de l’enquête PISA 2015 en Fédération Wallonie-Bruxelles. [The skills of 15-year-olds in science, mathematics and reading. Results of the PISA 2015 survey in the Wallonia-Brussels Federation]. Les Cahiers des Sciences de l’Éducation, v. 37, p. 1–188, 2017.
LANTIAN, A.; BAGNEUX, V.; DELOUVÉE, S.; GAUVRIT, N. Maybe a free thinker but not a critical one: High conspiracy belief is associated with low critical thinking ability. Applied Cognitive Psychology, v. 35, n. 3, p. 674–684, 2021. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3790
LANTIAN, A.; MULLER, D.; NURRA, C.; DOUGLAS, K. M. Measuring belief in conspiracy theories: Validation of a French and English single-item scale. International Review of Social Psychology, v. 29, n. 1, p. 1–14, 2016. https://doi.org/10.5334/irsp.8
LIEURY, A.; LORANT, S. Encyclopedic Memory: Long term memory capacity for knowledge vocabulary in middle school. International Journal of Educational Psychology, v. 2, n. 1, p. 56–80, 2013. https://doi.org/10.4471/ijep.2013.18
LIMA, I. G. d.; HYPOLITO, Á. M. Escola sem Partido: Análise de uma rede conservadora na educação. [Non-partisan school: An Analysis of a Conservative Network in Education]. Práxis Educativa, v. 15, 2020. https://doi.org/10.5212/PraxEduc.v.15.15290.053
Lionço, T.; Mattos, A. R. Ensino do criacionismo e da bíblia nas escolas brasileiras: Análise de proposições legislativas atuais [Teaching creationism and the bible in Brazilian schools: Analysis of current legislative proposals]. Estudos e Pesquisas em Psicologia, v. 21, n. 4, p. 1352–1373, 2021. https://doi.org/10.12957/epp.2021.63944
MANCOSU, M.; VASSALLO, S.; VEZZONI, C. Believing in conspiracy theories: evidence from an exploratory analysis of Italian survey data. South European Society and Politics, v. 22, n. 3, p. 327–344, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1080/13608746.2017.1359894
MCGOEY, L. The unknowers: How strategic ignorance rules the world. London: Zed Books, 2019.
MOCAN, N. H.; POGORELOVA, L. Compulsory schooling laws and formation of beliefs: Education, religion and superstition. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, v. 142, p. 509-539, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2017.07.005.
MORAIS, J.; BERTELSON, P.; CARY, L.; ALEGRIA, J. Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, v. 24, p. 45–64, 1986. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90004-1
MORAIS, J.; KOLINSKY, R. Seeing thought: A cultural cognitive tool. Journal of Cultural Cognitive Science, v. 5, p. 181–228, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41809-020-00059-0.
OECD. PISA for development assessment and analytical framework: Reading, mathematics and science. OECD Publishing, 2018.
OECD. PISA 2018 Results (Volume I): What students know and can do. Paris: PISA, OECD Publishing, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1787/5f07c754-en.
ONG, W. J. Orality and literacy: The technologizing of the word. New York, NY: Methuen, 1982.
ORTEGA, F.; ORSINI, M. Governing COVID-19 without government in Brazil: Ignorance, neoliberal authoritarianism, and the collapse of public health leadership. Global Public Health, v. 15, n. 9, p. 1257-1277, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1080/17441692.2020.1795223
PACHECO, L. P.; HÜBNER, L. C. What is the relationship between fake news’ power and deep reading comprehension level? Signo, v. X, 2022.
PANTAZI, M.; HALE, S.; KLEIN, O. Social and cognitive aspects of the vulnerability to political misinformation. Political Psychology, v. 42, n. S1, p. 267–304, 2021. https://doi.org/10.1111/pops.12797
PENNYCOOK, G.; RAND, D. G. Lazy, not biased: Susceptibility to partisan fake news is
better explained by lack of reasoning than by motivated reasoning. Cognition, v. 188, p. 39–
, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.06.011
PORCARI, J.; HAZUGA, R.; FOSTER, C.; DOBERSTEIN, S.; BECKER, J.; KLINE, D.; MICKSCHL, T.; DODGE, C. Can the Power Balance® bracelet improve balance, flexibility, strength, and power? Journal of Sports Science & Medicine, v. 10, p. 230–231, 2011.
ROOZENBEEK, J.; SCHNEIDER, C. R.; DRYHURST, S.; KERR, J.; FREEMAN, A. L. J.; RECCHIA, G.; VAN DER BLES, A. M.; VAN DER LINDEN, S. Susceptibility to misinformation about COVID-19 around the world. Royal Society Open Science, v. 7, n. 10, 201199, 2020. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.201199
SCHLEICHER, A. PISA 2018: Insights and Interpretations. OECD Publishing, 2018.
SCHOLES, R. J.; WILLIS, B. J. Language and literacy. Journal of Literary Semantics, v.16, p. 3–11, 1987a.
SCHOLES, R. J.; WILLIS, B. J. Age and education in oral language skills. Developmental Neuropsychology, v. 3, p. 239–248, 1987b.
SILVA, H.; PRADO, I. G. O. Creationism and intelligent design: presence in the Brazil educational policy. Procedia Social and Behavioural Sciences, vol. 2, p. 5260–5264, 2010. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.856.
SMALLE, E. H. M.; SZMALEC, A.; BOGAERTS, L.; PAGE, M. P. A.; NARANG, V.; MISRA, D. Literacy improves short-term serial recall of spoken verbal but not visuospatial items - evidence from illiterate and literate adults. Cognition, v. 185, p. 144–150, 2019. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2019.01.012.
SWAMI, V.; STIEGER, S.; PIETSCHNIG, J.; NADER, I. W.; VORACEK, M. Using more than 10% of our brains: examining belief in science-related myths from an individual differences perspective. Learning and Individual Differences, v. 22, p. 404–408, 2012. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.12.005.
SWAMI, V.; THORN, L. ; HUSBANDS, D.; TRAN, U.S.; NADER, I.W.; VON NORDHEIM, L.; PIETSCHNIG, J.; STIEGER, S.; VORACEK, M. Are the scope and nature of psychology properly understood? An examination of belief in myths of popular psychology among university students. In Columbus (Ed.), Advances in Psychology Research. Volume 101 (pp. 9–32). Hauppage, NY.: Nova Science Publishers, 2015.
TODOS PELA EDUCAÇÃO. Nota técnica: Impactos da pandemia na alfabetização de crianças. Todos pela Educação, 2021. [Technical Note: Impacts of the pandemic on children’s literacy. All for Education]. Available at: https://todospelaeducacao.org.br/wordpress/wp-content/uploads/2022/02/digital-nota-tecnica-alfabetizacao-1.pdf. Consulted on August 15, 2022.
UNESCO. Records of the General Conference, 20th session, v. 1. Paris: UNESCO, 1979.
USCINSKI, J. E., PARENT, J. M. American conspiracy theories. New York: Oxford University Press, 2014.
VÁGVÖLGYI, R.; COLDEA, A.; DRESLER, T.; SCHRADER, J.; NUERK, H.-C. A review about functional illiteracy: Definition, cognitive, linguistic, and numerical aspects. Frontiers in Psychology, v. 7:1617, 2016. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01617.
VAN PROOIJEN, J.-W. Why education predicts decreased belief in conspiracy theories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, v. 31, p. 50-58, 2017. https://doi.org/10.1002/acp.3301
VAN PROOIJEN, J.-W; LIGTHART, J.; ROSEMA, S.; XU, Y. The entertainment value of conspiracy theories. British Journal of Psychology, v. 113 n. 1, p. 25–48, 2022. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjop.12522
WARDLE, C.; DERAKHSHAN, H. Information disorder: Toward an interdisciplinary framework for research and policy making. Council of Europe report, 2017)09. https://rm.coe.int/information-disorder-toward-an-interdisciplinary-framework-for-researc/168076277c
WARDLE, C.; GREASON, G.; KERWIN, J.; DIAS, N. Information Disorder: The Essential Glossary. 2018. https://firstdraftnews.org/wp-content/uploads/2018/07/infoDisorder_glossary.pdf
WOOD, M. J.; DOUGLAS, K. M.; SUTTON, R. M. Dead and alive: Beliefs in contradictory conspiracy theories. Social Psychological and Personality Science, v. 3, n. 6, p. 767–773, 2012. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611434786.
##submission.downloads##
Publicado
Cómo citar
Número
Sección
Licencia
Declaro(amos) que o artigo submetido é pessoal, inédito, não representando reprodução, ainda que parcial, de obras de terceiros, assumindo a responsabilidade por todas as colocações e conceitos emitidos, bem como também autorizo(amos) sua publicação pela revista Signo, Universidade de Santa Cruz do Sul. Declaro(amos) exonerar a APESC/UNISC de todas e quaisquer responsabilidades, e indenizá-la por perdas e danos que venha a sofrer em caso de contestação (da originalidade e dos conceitos e ideias); Declaro(amos), caso o artigo seja aceito e publicado pela revista Signo, a cedência e transferência de forma definitiva e perpétua, irrevogável e irretratável, para a APESC, dos seus direitos autorais patrimoniais referentes ao artigo denominado nesta declaração, para utilização da APESC em finalidade educacional. Concordo(amos) e estou(amos) ciente(s) de que a publicação eletrônica é de livre acesso, regida com uma Licença Os autores que publicam na Signo retêm os direitos autorais de seu trabalho, licenciando-o sob a Creative Commons Attribution License que permite que os artigos sejam reutilizados e redistribuídos sem restrições, desde que o trabalho original seja corretamente citado. A Signo é propriedade da Associação Pró-Ensino em Santa Cruz do Sul e hospeda na plataforma Open Journal System. Creative Commons Atribuição 4.0 Internacional.